Factors Contributing to Second Language Speaking Development: Cognitive Aspects and Theoretical Models

Authors

  • Essa Qurbi

    English Department, Najran University, Najran 66462, Saudi Arabia

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.30564/fls.v7i9.10683
Received: 24 June 2025 | Revised: 9 July 2025 | Accepted: 22 July 2025 | Published Online: 16 September 2025

Abstract

This paper presents  an overview of some of the cognitive and neurolinguistic factors influencing second language (L2) speech development, focusing on factors like the role of different types of memory, psychological aspects, and theoretical models of second language (L2) acquisition. It sheds light on aspects like central executive, phonological loop, and visuospatial sketchpad which are the three components of working memory contributing to the processing of linguistic input and output. It also covers factors that contribute to L2 speaking development such as attention mechanisms with respect to selective and flexible attention which both allow efficient use of language comprehension and production. On the other hand, this review reviews factors related to psycholinguistics such as lexical access, grammatical encoding, and phonological processing which are responsible for contributing to fluent L2 speech production. Input Processing Model and Speech Production Model are models that contribute towards understanding of the interaction between L2 learning and speech production. Furthermore, factors like affective conditions, such as motivation and anxiety, are discussed as well. Overall, the present  paper provides an overview of these psychological and cognitive factors that remarkably influence effective L2 speaking and development.

Keywords:

Second Language Speaking; Cognitive Aspects; Psycholinguistics; Speaking Development

References

[1] Kroll, J.F., Bialystok, E., 2022. Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 25(5), 497–514. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.799170

[2] Lenneberg, E.H., 1967. The biological foundations of language. Hospital Practice. 2(12), 59–67.

[3] Abutalebi, J., Green, D.W., 2016. Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: Neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 19(4), 689–698. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000225

[4] Ullman, M. T., 2001. The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(1), 37–69. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005204207369

[5] Linck, J.A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J.T., Bunting, M.F., 2014. Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 21, 861–883.

[6] Daneman, M., Carpenter, P.A., 1980. Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 19(4), 450–466. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6

[7] Juffs, A., Harrington, M., 2011. Aspects of working memory in L2 learning. Language Teaching. 44(2), 137–166. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0261444810000509

[8] Levelt, W.J.M., 1989. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA.

[9] Kormos, J., 2006. Speech Production and Second Language Acquisition. Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA.

[10] Horwitz, E.K., Horwitz, M.B., Cope, J., 1986. Foreign language classroom anxiety. The Modern Language Journal. 70(2), 125–132. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1986.tb05256.x

[11] Dörnyei, Z., 2005. The Psychology of the Language Learner: Individual Differences in Second Language Acquisition. Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA.

[12] Mateos-Aparicio, P., Rodríguez-Moreno, A., 2019. The impact of studying brain plasticity. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience. 13, 66.

[13] Costa, A., Sebastián-Gallés, N., 2014. How does the bilingual experience sculpt the brain? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 15(5), 336–345.

[14] Green, D.W., Abutalebi, J., 2013. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 25(5), 515–530. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377

[15] Birdsong, D., 1999. Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis. Routledge: London, UK. pp. 1–22.

[16] White, L., Genesee, F., 1996. How native is near-native? The issue of ultimate attainment in adult second language acquisition. Second Language Research. 12(3), 233–265.

[17] Bialystok, E., 2017. The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin. 143(3), 233.

[18] Korenar, M., Treffers-Daller, J., Pliatsikas, C., 2023. Dynamic effects of bilingualism on brain structure map onto general principles of experience-based neuroplasticity. Scientific Reports. 13(1), 3428.

[19] Marian, V., Shook, A., 2012. The cognitive benefits of being bilingual. Cerebrum: The Dana Forum on Brain Science. Available from: https://www.dana.org/article/the-cognitive-benefits-of-being-bilingual/ (cited 9 October 2024)

[20] Ellis, R., 2002. The Study of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.

[21] Segalowitz, N., 2010. Cognitive Bases of Second Language Fluency. Routledge: New York, NY, USA.

[22] DeKeyser, R., 2007. Skill acquisition theory. In: VanPatten, B., Williams, J. (eds.). Theories in Second Language Acquisition. Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA. pp. 97–113.

[23] Tavakoli, P., 2019. L2 speaking. In: Derwing, T.M., Munro, M.J., Thomson, R.I., (eds.). The Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition and Speaking. Routledge: New York, NY, USA. pp. 123–136.

[24] Just, M.A., Carpenter, P.A., 1992. A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review. 99(1), 122.

[25] Baddeley, A., 2003. Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 4(10), 829–839.

[26] Collette, F., van der Linden, M., 2002. Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 26(2), 105–125.

[27] Wen, Z., 2012. Working memory and second language learning. International Journal of Applied Linguistics. 22(1), 1–22.

[28] Bhatnagar, S., Singh, P., 2024. Role of visuospatial sketchpad in second language acquisition. Journal of Cognition and Culture. 24(1–2), 35–50.

[29] Baddeley, A.D., Hitch, G.J., 1974. Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation. 8, 47–89.

[30] Osaka, M., Osaka, N., 1992. Language-independent working memory as measured by Japanese and English reading span tests. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society. 30, 287–289.

[31] Daneman, M., 1991. Working memory as a predictor of verbal fluency. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 20(6), 445–464.

[32] Szmalec, A., Brysbaert, M., Duyck, W., 2012. Working memory and (second) language processing. In: Altarriba, J. (ed.). Memory, Language, and Bilingualism: Theoretical and Applied Approaches, Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK. pp. 74–94. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139035279.004

[33] Gass, S.M., Selinker, L., 2008. Second Language Acquisition: An Introductory Course, 3rd ed. Routledge: New York, NY, USA.

[34] Segalowitz, N., Frenkiel-Fishman, S., 2005. Attention control and ability level in a complex cognitive skill: Attention shifting and second-language proficiency. Memory & Cognition. 33(4), 644–653.

[35] Bialystok, E., Viswanathan, M., 2009. Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition. 112(3), 494–500.

[36] Kormos, J., 2011. Speech Production and Second Language Acquisition. Routledge: New York, NY, USA.

[37] De Bot, K., 1992. A bilingual production model: Levelt’s “speaking” model adapted. Applied Linguistics. 13(1), 1–24.

[38] Taft, M., 1990. Lexical processing of functionally constrained words. Journal of Memory and Language. 29(2), 245–257.

[39] Dahan, D., Magnuson, J.S., Tanenhaus, M.K., 2001. Time course of frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Psychology. 42(4), 317–367.

[40] Duyck, W., Vanderelst, D., Desmet, T., et al., 2008. The frequency effect in second-language visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 15, 850–855.

[41] Collins, A.M., Loftus, E.F., 1975. A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review. 82(6), 407.

[42] Arêas Da Luz Fontes, A.B., Schwartz, A.I., 2010. On a different plane: Cross-language effects on the conceptual representations of within-language homonyms. Language and Cognitive Processes. 25(4), 508–532.

[43] Schmitt, N., 2010. Researching Vocabulary: A Vocabulary Research Manual. Palgrave Macmillan: Basingstoke, UK.

[44] Bock, K., Levelt, W., Gernsbacher, M.A., 2002. Language production: Grammatical encoding. In: Psycholinguistics: Critical Concepts in Psychology, Vol. 5. pp. 405–452.

[45] Hartsuiker, R.J., Pickering, M.J., Veltkamp, E., 2004. Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science. 15(6), 409–414.

[46] Bock, J.K., 1986. Meaning, sound, and syntax: Lexical priming in sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 12(4), 575.

[47] Gass, S.M., Selinker, L., 1992. Language Transfer in Language Learning, Revised edition. John Benjamins Publishing: Amsterdam, Netherlands. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.5

[48] Gardner, R.C., Lambert, W.E., 1972. Attitude and Motivation in Second Language Learning. Newbury House: Rowley, MA, USA.

[49] Chen, T.Y., Chang, G.B., 2004. The relationship between foreign language anxiety and learning difficulties. Foreign Language Annals. 37(2), 279–289.

[50] Horwitz, E.K., 2001. Language anxiety and achievement. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics. 21, 112–126.

[51] Woodrow, L., 2006. Anxiety and speaking English as a second language. RELC Journal. 37(3), 308–328. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0033688206071315

[52] Gregersen, T., Horwitz, E.K., 2002. Language learning and perfectionism: Anxious and non-anxious language learners’ reactions to their own oral performance. The Modern Language Journal. 86(4), 562–570. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-4781.00161

[53] Phillips, E.M., 1992. The effects of language anxiety on students' oral test performance and attitudes. The Modern Language Journal. 76(1), 14–26. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2307/329894

[54] Price, M.L., 1991. The subjective experience of foreign language anxiety: Interviews with highly anxious students. In: Horwitz, E.K., Young D.J. (eds.). Language Anxiety: From Theory and Research to Classroom Implications. Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA. pp. 101–108

[55] Long, M.H., 1996. The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W.C. Ritchie, T.K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition, pp. 413–468. Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA.

[56] De Jong, N., Perfetti, C.A., 2011. Fluency training in the ESL classroom: An experimental study of fluency development and proceduralization. Language Learning. 61(2), 533–568. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00620.x

[57] Swain, M., 2000. The output hypothesis and beyond: Mediating acquisition through collaborative dialogue. In: Lantolf, J.P. (ed.). Sociocultural Theory and Second Language Learning. Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA. pp. 97–114.

[58] Wajnryb, R., 1990. Grammar Dictation. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.

Downloads

How to Cite

Qurbi, E. (2025). Factors Contributing to Second Language Speaking Development: Cognitive Aspects and Theoretical Models. Forum for Linguistic Studies, 7(9), 879–890. https://doi.org/10.30564/fls.v7i9.10683

Issue

Article Type

Review