Reading, Spelling, and Visual Lexical Decision in English: the Case of Spanish Children with Dyslexia

Authors

  • Martínez-García Cristina

    Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Universitat Oberta de Catalunya, Rambla del Poblenou, 156, Barcelona, 08018, Spain

  • Marina Vega-Harwood

    Language Psychology Laboratory - Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Plaza Feijoo, Oviedo, Asturias, Spain

  • Paz Suárez-Coalla

    Language Psychology Laboratory - Department of Psychology, University of Oviedo, Plaza Feijoo, Oviedo, Asturias, Spain

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.30564/fls.v6i4.6729
Received: 5 June 2024 | Revised: 28 June 2024 | Accepted: 5 July 2024 | Published Online: 5 September 2024

Abstract

The primary challenge that children with dyslexia face is the difficulty in learning the alphabetic code and forming orthographic representations of words. Consequently, their reading and writing abilities lack fluidity. Given factors, it is possible that children with dyslexia may encounter difficulties when learning English as a Foreign Language. This research investigates the significance of lexical frequency and orthographic rime consistency in the reading and spelling processes of English as a Foreign Language among Spanish children diagnosed with dyslexia. The study included 36 participants aged between 9 and 12, of whom 16 were clinically diagnosed with dyslexia. Three tasks were designed using 28 English words, varying in lexical frequency and orthographic consistency: word reading aloud, spelling to dictation, and visual lexical decision. Our analysis focused on accuracy, indicating that children diagnosed with dyslexia consistently exhibited poorer performance across all tasks compared to their counterparts. While lexical frequency exerted an influence on both groups, orthographic consistency exclusively impacted the control group. The findings suggest that Spanish children with dyslexia predominantly depend on lexical frequency for reading and spelling in English. In contrast, typically developing children, in addition to frequency, also show sensitivity to orthographic consistency skills.

Keywords:

developmental dyslexia, EFL, lexical frequency, orthographic consistency, Spanish children

References

Afonso, O., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2015). Spelling impairments in Spanish dyslexic adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00466

Afonso, O., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2019). Writing impairments in Spanish children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 53(2), 109–119. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219419876255

Alegría, J., & Carrillo, M. (2014). Learning to spell words in Spanish: a comparative analysis / La escritura de palabras en castellano: un análisis comparativo. Estudios De Psicología/Estudios De Psicología, 35(3), 476–501. https://doi.org/10.1080/02109395.2014.978544

Álvarez-Cañizo, M., Afonso, O., & Suárez-Coalla, P. (2023). Writing proficiency in English as L2 in Spanish children with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 73(1), 130–147. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-023-00278-4

Angelelli, P., Notarnicola, A., Judica, A., Zoccolotti, P., & Luzzatti, C. (2010). Spelling impairments in Italian dyslexic children: Phenomenological changes in primary school. Cortex, 46(10), 1299–1311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.015

Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D. H., & Yap, M. J. (2004). Visual word recognition of Single-Syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 133(2), 283–316. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.283

Barca, L., Burani, C., Di Filippo, G., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Italian developmental dyslexic and proficient readers: Where are the differences? Brain and Language, 98(3), 347–351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.05.001

Bonin, P., Laroche, B., & Perret, C. (2016). Locus of word frequency effects in spelling to dictation: Still at the orthographic level! Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(11), 1814–1820. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000278

Borleffs, E., Maassen, B. a. M., Lyytinen, H., & Zwarts, F. (2019). Cracking the code: The Impact of Orthographic Transparency and Morphological-Syllabic Complexity on reading and Developmental Dyslexia. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02534

Brysbaert, M., Lagrou, E., & Stevens, M. (2017). Visual word recognition in a second language: A test of the lexical entrenchment hypothesis with lexical decision times. Bilingualism, 20(3), 530–548. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728916000353

Brysbaert, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E. (2018). The Word Frequency Effect in Word Processing: An updated review. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(1), 45–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417727521

Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E. (2016). The impact of word prevalence on lexical decision times: Evidence from the Dutch Lexicon Project 2. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 42(3), 441–458. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000159

Burani, C., Marcolini, S., & Stella, G. (2002). How early does morpholexical reading develop in readers of a shallow orthography? Brain and Language, 81(1–3), 568–586. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2548

Caravolas, M., Volín, J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92(2), 107–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.04.003

Carioti, D., Masia, M. F., Travellini, S., & Berlingeri, M. (2021). Orthographic depth and developmental dyslexia: a meta-analytic study. Annals of Dyslexia, 71(3), 399–438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-021-00226-0

Chateau, D., & Jared, D. (2003). Spelling–sound consistency effects in disyllabic word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(2), 255–280. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0749-596x(02)00521-1

Chee, Q. W., Chow, K. J., Yap, M. J., & Goh, W. D. (2020). Consistency norms for 37,677 english words. Behavior Research Methods, 52(6), 2535–2555. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01391-7

Cook, V. & Ryan, D. (2016). The Routledge Handbook of the English Writing System. New York: Routledge.

Cuetos, F., & Suárez-Coalla, P. (2009). From grapheme to word in reading acquisition in Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(4), 583–601. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716409990038

Cuetos, F., Arribas, D., Suárez‐Coalla, P., & Martínez-García, C. (2020). PROLEXIA. Diagnóstico y Detección Temprana de la Dislexia.

Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49(2), 222–251. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543049002222

Davies, R., Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2013). Lexical and sub-lexical effects on accuracy, reaction time and response duration: impaired and typical word and pseudoword reading in a transparent orthography. Reading & Writing, 26(5), 721–738. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9388-1

De Luca, M., Barca, L., Burani, C., & Zoccolotti, P. (2008). The effect of word length and other sublexical, lexical, and semantic variables on developmental reading deficits. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 21(4), 227–235. https://doi.org/10.1097/wnn.0b013e318190d162

De Simone, E., Beyersmann, E., Mulatti, C., Mirault, J., & Schmalz, X. (2021). Order among chaos: Cross-linguistic differences and developmental trajectories in pseudoword reading aloud using pronunciation Entropy. PloS One, 16(5), e0251629. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251629

Diependaele, K., Lemhöfer, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). The word frequency effect in first- and second-language word recognition: A lexical entrenchment account. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(5), 843–863. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.720994

Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(1), 116–124. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03195503

Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis☆. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 787–814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.001

Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological Skills and Learning to Read. Hove: Erlbaum.

Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., Dalton, L., & Schneider, W. (2001). Pseudohomophone effects and phonological recoding procedures in reading development in English and German. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(4), 648–664. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2790

Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., Dalton, L., & Schneider, W. (2003). Nonword reading across orthographies: How flexible is the choice of reading units? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(2), 235–247. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716403000134

Grainger, J., Bouttevin, S., Truc, C., Bastien, M., & Ziegler, J. (2003). Word superiority, pseudoword superiority, and learning to read: A comparison of dyslexic and normal readers. Brain and Language, 87(3), 432–440. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00145-7

Guinet, E., & Kandel, S. (2010). Ductus: A software package for the study of handwriting production. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 326–332. https://doi.org/10.3758/brm.42.1.326

Hayes, H., Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2006). Children use vowels to help them spell consonants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94(1), 27–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.11.001

Hulme, C. J., & Snowling, M. J. (2009). Developmental disorders of language learning and cognition. Blackwell/Wiley.

Hulme, C., Snowling, M., Caravolas, M., & Carroll, J. (2005). Phonological skills are (Probably) one cause of success in learning to read: A comment on Castles and Coltheart. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(4), 351–365. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0904_2

Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The Reading Process is Different for Different Orthographies: The Orthographic Depth Hypothesis. In Orthography, Phonology, Morphology, and Meaning (p. 14).

Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Wuggy: A multilingual pseudoword generator. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 627–633. https://doi.org/10.3758/brm.42.3.627

Lindgren, S. D., De Renzi, E., & Richman, L. C. (1985). Cross-National Comparisons of Developmental dyslexia in Italy and the United States. Child Development, 56(6), 1404. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130460

Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-003-0001-9

Martinet, C., Valdois, S., & Fayol, M. (2004). Lexical orthographic knowledge develops from the beginning of literacy acquisition. Cognition, 91(2), B11–B22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.09.002

Martínez-García, C., Cuetos, F., Pérez-Litago, U. & Suárez-Coalla, P. in preparation. Lexical frequency database of English words for Spanish children.

Martínez-García, C., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2019). Development of orthographic representations in Spanish children with dyslexia: the influence of previous semantic and phonological knowledge. Annals of Dyslexia, 69(2), 186–203. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-019-00178-6

Pérez-Litago, U., Rojas-Guerra, J.M., Martínez-García, C., & Suárez-Coalla, P. (submitted). Languaje skills of adults with dyslexia in English as a foreign language.

Protopapas, A. (2007). Check Vocal: A program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMDX. Behavior Research Methods, 39(4), 859–862. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03192979

R Development Core Team. (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Richlan, F. (2014). Functional neuroanatomy of developmental dyslexia: the role of orthographic depth. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00347

Schmalz, X., Beyersmann, E., Cavalli, E., & Marinus, E. (2016). Unpredictability and complexity of print-to-speech correspondences increase reliance on lexical processes: more evidence for the orthographic depth hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(6), 658–672. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2016.1182172

Serrano, F., & Defior, S. (2008). Dyslexia speed problems in a transparent orthography. Annals of Dyslexia 58, 81–95. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-008-0013-6

Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94(2), 143–174. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712603321661859

Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., & Nation, K. (2020). Defining and understanding dyslexia: past, present and future. Oxford Review of Education, 46(4), 501–513. https://doi.org/10.1080/03054985.2020.1765756

Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 24–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.24

Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2012). Reading strategies in Spanish developmental dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 62(2), 71–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-011-0064-y

Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2015). Reading difficulties in Spanish adults with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 65, 33–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-015-0101-3

Suárez-Coalla, P., Álvarez‐Cañizo, M., & Cuetos, F. (2016). Orthographic learning in Spanish children. Journal of Research in Reading, 39(3), 292–311. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12043

Suárez-Coalla, P., Castejón, L., Vega-Harwood, M., & Martínez-García, C. (2023). English reading performance by Spanish speaking children: A phonologically or semantically mediated pathway? Reading & Writing. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10497-1

Suárez-Coalla, P., Hevia‐Tuero, C., Martínez‐García, C., & Afonso, O. (2024). Spanish children spelling in English as a foreign language: Central and peripheral processes. Journal of Research in Reading. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12456

Suárez-Coalla, P., Martínez-García, C., & Carnota, A. (2020). Reading in English as a foreign language by Spanish children with dyslexia. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00019

Suárez-Coalla, P., Ramos, S., Álvarez-Cañizo, M., & Cuetos, F. (2014). Orthographic learning in dyslexic Spanish children. Annals of Dyslexia, 64(2), 166–181. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-014-0092-5

Swanson, H. L., & Hsieh, C. (2009). Reading Disabilities in Adults: A Selective Meta-Analysis of the Literature. Review of Educational Research, 79(4), 1362–1390. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654309350931

Takashima, A., Hulzink, I., Wagensveld, B., & Verhoeven, L. (2016). Emergence of representations through repeated training on pronouncing novel letter combinations leads to efficient reading. Neuropsychologia, 89, 14–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.014

Wang, H., Castles, A., & Nickels, L. (2012). Word regularity affects orthographic learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 856–864. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.672996

Wimmer, H., & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition, 51(1), 91–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90010-8

Yap, M. J., & Balota, D. A. (2009). Visual word recognition of multisyllabic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(4), 502–529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.02.001

Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading Acquisition, Developmental dyslexia, and Skilled Reading across Languages: A Psycholinguistic Grain Size Theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3

Downloads

How to Cite

Cristina, M.-G., Vega-Harwood, M., & Suárez-Coalla, P. (2024). Reading, Spelling, and Visual Lexical Decision in English: the Case of Spanish Children with Dyslexia. Forum for Linguistic Studies, 6(4), 28–41. https://doi.org/10.30564/fls.v6i4.6729